Action

Use low intensity lighting

How is the evidence assessed?
  • Effectiveness
    not assessed
  • Certainty
    not assessed
  • Harms
    not assessed

Study locations

Key messages

  • Four studies evaluated the effects of using low intensity lighting on reptile populations. Three studies were in the USA1-3 and one was in Malaysia4.

COMMUNITY RESPONSE (0 STUDIES)

POPULATION RESPONSE (0 STUDIES)

BEHAVIOUR (4 STUDIES)

  • Behaviour change (4 studies): One replicated, controlled study in the USA1 found that reducing the intensity of light sources did not improve loggerhead turtle hatchling seaward orientation. One replicated, site comparison study in Malaysia4 found that green turtle hatchlings in low and moderate ambient artificial light took more direct crawl routes to the sea than hatchlings released in high ambient artificial light. One replicated, controlled study in the USA3 found that in laboratory trials, loggerhead and green turtle hatchlings showed reduced preference for lower intensity light sources. One replicated, site comparison study in the USA2 found mixed effects of embedding streetlights in the road on seaward orientation of loggerhead turtle hatchlings compared to overhead lighting depending on shading by shrubs and weather and lunar phase.

About key messages

Key messages provide a descriptive index to studies we have found that test this intervention.

Studies are not directly comparable or of equal value. When making decisions based on this evidence, you should consider factors such as study size, study design, reported metrics and relevance of the study to your situation, rather than simply counting the number of studies that support a particular interpretation.

Supporting evidence from individual studies

  1. A replicated, controlled study (years not provided) on a beach in Florida, USA (Witherington & Bjorndal 1991) found that using low rather than high intensity lighting did not improve loggerhead turtle Caretta caretta hatchling orientation seawards in any of five types of commercially-available artificial light types, but that one of 10 lighting treatments showed similar ranges of orientation to no lighting. Only high intensity yellow tinted incandescent lighting did not affect hatchling orientation compared when no lighting was used (results presented as crawl angles, see paper for details). Only low intensity low-pressure sodium vapour light did not affect the variation in crawl angle (results presented as crawl angles, see paper for details). Hatchlings in trials with white light sources (low and high intensity) showed the worst seaward orientation (results presented as crawl angles, see paper for details). Five commercially available lights were trialled at low and high intensities: high-pressure sodium vapour, low-pressure sodium vapour, yellow-tinted incandescent, red-tinted incandescent and white quartz. Trials were conducted at night by releasing 30 hatchlings/trial (from 30 different clutches) into the centre of an 8 m diameter sand arena divided into 32 segments, with segment one closest to the sea (0°) and a light positioned 4 m from the eighth segment (90°). After five minutes, the segment location of hatchlings was recorded. Trials were also carried out with no lighting.

    Study and other actions tested
  2. A replicated, site comparison study in 2001 on a sandy beach in Florida, USA (Bertolotti & Salmon 2005) found that when street lighting was embedded in the road, loggerhead turtle Caretta caretta hatchling orientation was more often seawards compared to when overhead lighting was used, though results depended on whether there was a full moon or new moon. Hatchling orientation was similar between embedded and overhead lighting conditions (seawards) in both unshaded and shaded sites during full moons. During the new moon, hatchlings moved seawards at all three sites when embedded lights were used, whereas with overhead lighting, the direction of movement was seaward in shaded areas, but more mixed in unshaded areas. With embedded lights and cloud cover, fewer turtles oriented directly eastwards compared to when there was no cloud cover. The nesting beach was parallel to a road with streetlights. In July–September 2001, loggerhead turtle hatchling orientation was compared between embedded lighting (LED lights in road studs at 9 m intervals) and overhead lighting (150W high-pressure sodium vapour angled away from the beach, 7.5–9 m high on wooden poles at 60–100 m intervals) in two unshaded sites and one site shaded by shrubs. Newly emerged hatchlings were placed in a 4 m diameter arena and exit direction from the arena was recorded. Trials were carried out 2–3 times for each site, lighting condition and moon phase (24 hatchlings from two or more nests/trial, sourced from 76 total nests).

    Study and other actions tested
  3. A replicated, controlled study in 2000–2001 in a laboratory in Florida, USA (Sella et al. 2006) found that lowering the intensity of a light source led to lower preference for moving towards that light source in laboratory trials with loggerhead Caretta caretta or green Chelonia mydas turtle hatchlings. Hatchlings preferentially crawled towards unfiltered light over orange or red light in four of four trials, but preference for unfiltered light decreased as its intensity decreased (from 2 of 4 trials to 0 of 4 trials with decreasing intensity). At the lowest intensity, turtles showed preference for the filtered light in two of four trials. In comparisons between orange and red filtered light, turtles showed no preference in six of eight trials, but preferred red light when the orange light intensity was reduced to its lowest level (2 of 2 trials). Hatchlings were presented with a choice of two lights to crawl towards. High pressure sodium vapour lights were covered with an orange, red or no filter and the less filtered light in each comparison was tested at four intensity levels. The highest intensity used matched that of a beach adjacent streetlight located 40 or 60 m from a nest, and intensity was reduced using a neutral density filter. In 2000–2001, hatchlings were obtained from two beaches, with 25 used in each trial. Each hatchling was used in only one trial and then released in to the wild.

    Study and other actions tested
  4. A replicated, site comparison study in 2005 on a sandy beach on the east coast of Terengganu, Peninsular Malaysia (van de Merwe et al. 2013) found that green turtle Chelonia mydas hatchlings from a hatchery released in low and moderate ambient artificial light took a more direct route to the sea than hatchlings released in high ambient artificial light. Green turtle hatchlings released in low and moderate artificial ambient light dispersed at an angle similar to a direct line towards to the sea (62–76° dispersal angle), while hatchlings released in high ambient light dispersed at a different angle to the most direct route (148° dispersal angle, see original paper for information about directionality). In July–October 2005, sea-finding behaviours were tested on 30 hatchlings from each of 14 hatchery nests (420 hatchlings) under three different ambient lighting scenarios–high ambient light (300 m north of hatchery), moderate ambient light (directly in front of hatchery) and low ambient light (500 m south of hatchery; 10 hatchlings/nest/lighting scenario). All trials were conducted in an 8m wide circular sand arena 20 m from the sea with hatchlings placed under a bucket in the centre before release. The angle of dispersal for each hatchling was calculated using a compass from the centre to the point where the hatchling exited the arena and compared to the angle of the direct route to the sea.

    Study and other actions tested
Please cite as:

Sainsbury K.A., Morgan W.H., Watson M., Rotem G., Bouskila A., Smith R.K. & Sutherland W.J. (2021) Reptile Conservation: Global Evidence for the Effects of Interventions for reptiles. Conservation Evidence Series Synopsis. University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK.

Where has this evidence come from?

List of journals searched by synopsis

All the journals searched for all synopses

Reptile Conservation

This Action forms part of the Action Synopsis:

Reptile Conservation
Reptile Conservation

Reptile Conservation - Published 2021

Reptile synopsis

What Works 2021 cover

What Works in Conservation

What Works in Conservation provides expert assessments of the effectiveness of actions, based on summarised evidence, in synopses. Subjects covered so far include amphibians, birds, mammals, forests, peatland and control of freshwater invasive species. More are in progress.

More about What Works in Conservation

Download free PDF or purchase
The Conservation Evidence Journal

The Conservation Evidence Journal

An online, free to publish in, open-access journal publishing results from research and projects that test the effectiveness of conservation actions.

Read the latest volume: Volume 21

Go to the CE Journal

Discover more on our blog

Our blog contains the latest news and updates from the Conservation Evidence team, the Conservation Evidence Journal, and our global partners in evidence-based conservation.


Who uses Conservation Evidence?

Meet some of the evidence champions

Endangered Landscape ProgrammeRed List Champion - Arc Kent Wildlife Trust The Rufford Foundation Save the Frogs - Ghana Mauritian Wildlife Supporting Conservation Leaders
Sustainability Dashboard National Biodiversity Network Frog Life The international journey of Conservation - Oryx Cool Farm Alliance UNEP AWFA Bat Conservation InternationalPeople trust for endangered species Vincet Wildlife Trust